Click Banner For More Info See All Sponsors

So Long and Thanks for All the Fish!

This site is now closed permanently to new posts.
We recommend you use the new Townsy Cafe!

Click anywhere but the link to dismiss overlay!

Results 1 to 2 of 2

  • Share this thread on:
  • Follow: No Email   
  • Thread Tools
  1. TopTop #1
    Glia's Avatar
    Glia
     

    The Origins of Bullying

    This article brings two things to mind:

    1. the recent request made of Ubaru that she clarify whether or not she is a paid blogger (full disclosure: a similar question had crossed my mind).

    2. the social conformity, control and intimidation elements of genital cutting on children. It is bullying and peer pressure on a trans-generational basis, and the bottom line is that it is all about control. Everything else is just rationalization.

    ----------
    The Origins of Bullying
    By Hogan Sherrow
    Scientific American
    December 15, 2011
    https://blogs.scientificamerican.com...s-of-bullying/

    Late on a Saturday night in September, a 14-year old boy
    named Jamey Rodemeyer, who had been the target of
    bullying from fellow students at Williamsville North
    High School in Buffalo New York, took his life. Just
    hours before he killed himself, Jamey left the last of
    his numerous messages online talking about the pain he
    had been dealing with for a long time. Jamey's suicide
    was a terrible, extreme reaction to being bullied, and
    tragically, his was not an unusual case. According to
    some reports there were as many as 10 teen suicides in
    the month of September this year, in the United States,
    that were linked to bullying. Violent reactions by teens
    to being bullied are not new. It was boys that were
    bullied and ostracized that committed the high school
    shootings that plagued the US in the 1990's. From those
    mass slaughters to the present day rash of suicides,
    bullying is taking a violent toll on the youth of
    America.

    The response to this crisis in the United States has
    been efforts at the local, regional and Federal
    (stopbullying.gov) levels to combat bullying and its
    impacts. Working groups, task forces and new policies
    have all been established, with the hopes of halting the
    spread of the social scourge that is bullying. While it
    is clear that bullying has become a critical issue both
    within US schools and the social systems navigated by
    America's youth, what is less clear is where its origins
    lie. It's easy to get consumed with the impacts and
    immediate causes of bullying in the US, and to ignore
    where bullying stems from. However, understanding the
    origins of bullying is critical. Without the deep
    understanding the origins of a behavior provide, efforts
    to prevent bullying will continue to fail.

    To understand where bullying comes from, we have to look
    at the phenomenon on multiple levels. The first step is
    to define bullying. Bullying is a behavior that is often
    difficult to measure, but is something that we all think
    we know when we see it. Many of us have experienced
    bullying first-hand, and most of us have witnessed it at
    some point. However, to study any trait or
    characteristic, we must first define what it is, and
    bullying is no exception. According to psychological
    sources, bullying is a specific type of aggression in
    which (1) the behavior is intended to harm or disturb,
    (2) the behavior occurs repeatedly over time, and (3)
    there is an imbalance of power, with a more powerful
    person or group attacking a less powerful one. This
    asymmetry of power may be physical or psychological, and
    the aggressive behavior may be verbal (eg, name-calling,
    threats), physical (eg, hitting), or psychological (eg,
    rumors, shunning/exclusion). The key elements of this
    definition are that multiple means can be employed by
    the bully or bullies, intimidation is the goal, and
    bullying can happen on a one-on-one or group basis
    (Nansel et al, 2001).

    Now that we've established a definition for bullying,
    there are two distinct levels of analysis that will shed
    light on the behavior and its origins. The first level
    of analysis is to determine if bullying is a cultural
    phenomenon. In other words, is bullying unique to US
    society, or is it widespread across different cultures,
    from different parts of the world? If bullying is
    widespread and found throughout different societies, we
    have to consider that it has a deeper origin than
    present cultural conditions. In short, we can deepen our
    analysis of the behavior. Bullying is, in fact,
    widespread and not restricted to American society, but
    instead is found across the globe (Smith et al, 2002).
    From hunter/gatherer groups (Boehm, 2000) to post-
    industrial Japan, bullying is ubiquitous across human
    cultures.

    A 2005 multinational study that spanned 28 countries
    across North America and Europe revealed how commonplace
    bullying is and how consistent its effects are (Due et
    al, 2005). Due et al (2005) used 12 physical and
    psychological symptoms associated with being bullied to
    measure the effects of this behavior on the youth in the
    study. They found that the amount of bullying
    experienced by kids in those 28 countries varied
    greatly, with the least severe happening among girls in
    Sweden and the most severe among boys in Lithuania.
    However, despite the variation in the amount of
    bullying, there were no countries where bullying was
    completely absent. Further, Due et al reported that,

    "There was a consistent, strong and graded association
    between bullying and each of 12 physical and
    psychological symptoms among adolescents in all 28
    countries." (Due et al, 2005).

    No matter where you go in the world, from the Mbuti of
    Central Africa (Turnbull, 1961) to Suburban children in
    the United States (Wang et al, 2009) there are
    individuals and groups that target others with tactics
    designed to intimidate, coerce or harm them. In some
    cases bullying is used to maintain social order and
    ensure that no one acquires too much dominance, status
    or personal power. In other cases, bullying is harmful
    and used to injure others physically, emotionally or
    socially. These scenarios are two sides of the same
    coin, and one can easily metamorphose into the other if
    the power dynamics become skewed in one direction or the
    other. Despite the variation in the amount and intention
    of bullying across human cultures one thing is clear,
    bullying is everywhere. The universality of bullying
    across human societies indicates that this is a species-
    typical human behavior that has little to do with the
    cultures people live in. Bullying, it seems is part of
    our normal behavioral repertoire, it is part of the
    human condition.

    Human universals are important to our understanding of
    the evolution of behavior in our species (Cosmides &
    Tooby, 1990). Despite our extensive knowledge of the
    human fossil record, we can't directly observe the
    behaviors of our ancestors. While fossils and ecological
    reconstructions provide some insights into behavior,
    modern human and other primates provide important clues
    as well. When we see modern human behaviors that are
    universal in nature, it tells us that these behaviors
    have their origins deep in our evolutionary history. At
    the very least universal behaviors evolved early on in
    our species prehistory and they were almost certainly
    present before humans began migrating around the world
    and separating into different, sometimes isolated ethnic
    groups. Bullying is one such behavior. It was there in
    the hot, seasonal grasslands of southern Africa when the
    first members of our species took their seminal steps
    and spoke the original human language, and it has been
    with us ever since. However, universal behaviors can
    pre-date a species origin, having been inherited from a
    previous ancestor. That's what the next level of
    analysis can tell us about bullying and its origins.

    The second level of analysis is to determine if bullying
    is unique to our species. To do this, we need to look at
    whether or not bullying is present in other species.
    Using the definition provided by above, this is a tall
    order, because that definition requires knowledge of
    intentionality. Intentions are difficult to identify in
    other animals because no matter how many times you ask
    them why they did something, they don't answer (at least
    I've never gotten an answer from them). However, if we
    employ the "key elements" of bullying as Nansel defines
    them, we don't need to know the intentions of
    individuals, we just have to determine if the purpose of
    a particular behavior was to intimidate. By using
    intimidation as our litmus for bullying, we can, at the
    very least, test for bullying-like behaviors in other
    animals, including other primates. If other primates
    engage in bullying-like behaviors, we have to consider
    the distinct possibility that bullying itself is deeply
    rooted in our evolutionary history and predates our own
    species.

    When bullying is considered across animals, there is
    ample evidence that many other animals, including other
    primates, engage in bullying-like behaviors. Rats and
    mice are commonly used as models for social stress
    during different life phases, including adolescence.
    Studies on these common laboratory rodents indicate that
    social stress, experienced when one individual
    repeatedly attacks another or takes resources from them,
    has immediate and lasting impacts (Kinsey et al, 2007;
    Vidal et al, 2011). Rats who suffered from bullying-like
    behaviors were less likely to drink water or consume
    other resources (Vidal et al, 2011). Mice that suffered
    repeated social defeats were more anxious and
    experienced changes in brain chemistry (Kinsey et al,
    2007). Bullying-like behaviors extend beyond rodents,
    and labs, appearing in many species, including other
    primates.

    Bullying-like behaviors are found in every major group
    of primates, and can sometimes be severe. Among baboons,
    one of the best-known, non-human primates in the world,
    bullying-like behaviors are common. Baboons are common
    throughout sub-Saharan Africa and many species live in
    female-centered societies that are held together by
    matrilineal bonds that span multiple generations. Groups
    of related females work together to compete over
    resources and in doing so regularly gang up on females
    from other matrilines (Altmann, 1980). Female baboons
    have large canines (though nowhere near as large as
    their male counterparts) and their fights can be intense
    and, occasionally, dangerous. Females who regularly lose
    fights and are low ranking are more stressed and have
    lower reproductive success than their higher-ranking
    group-mates (Sapolsky, 1987). While female baboons are
    not always bully-like toward one another, they
    frequently use intimidation and aggression to modify the
    behaviors of others and to get resources from them
    (Seyfarth, 1976).

    Bullying-like behaviors are not restricted to female
    primates. Chimpanzees live in communities with many
    males and females and males live in the groups their
    born into their entire lives. Males also form dominance
    relationships with each other based on physical power
    and friendships, which they use in competition over
    mates. Male chimpanzees regularly intimidate each other
    with bluffs, displays, charges and aggression, which can
    range from making another male move from a resting spot
    to physical violence. One of the areas I focus on in my
    research is the development of behavior in male
    chimpanzees, paying particular attention to adolescence.
    Adolescence is a time of great change and uncertainty
    for male chimpanzees, when they leave their mothers and
    enter into the adult male social world. When they do
    that they enter a world of constant posturing and
    networking that threatens to erupt into violence at any
    moment. Much like their human cousins, adolescent male
    chimpanzees begin at the bottom of the male dominance
    hierarchy (Goodall, 1986) and have to demonstrate their
    value as a friend and ally, while growing and putting on
    muscle mass in order to move up the hierarchy. Because
    adolescent males are smaller, weaker, less experienced
    and have to challenge other males in order to become
    competitive, they make attractive targets for older
    males, and older adolescents and adults regularly attack
    them (Sherrow, 2008). In short, adolescent males are
    almost continually bullied as they attempt to join the
    male social world.

    In most cases the bullying-like behaviors experienced by
    male chimpanzees are temporary and relatively harmless.
    The most common form of intimidation involves a dominant
    male puffing himself up, with all of his hair standing
    on end, and walking toward or by another male. This is
    usually enough to compel the subordinate, or lower
    ranking, male to pant grunt (a short "uhh, uhh, uhh"
    vocalization which is repeated several times and serves
    to recognize the dominance of another chimpanzee), don a
    fear grimace and put their hand out in a palm up begging
    gesture. However, if two males are close in rank or a
    male fails to adhere to social norms within the
    community, bullying-like behaviors can become more
    intense and, on occasion, dangerous.

    One of the reasons bullying-like behaviors can become so
    dangerous among male chimpanzees is that they regularly
    gang up on each other during aggressive interactions in
    what are called coalitions. On three different
    occasions, researchers at three different field sites,
    observed coalitions of adult male chimpanzees attack and
    kill a male from their group, apparently because they
    did not adhere to the social norms of the community
    (Fawcett & Muhumza, 2000; Nishida, 1996; Watts, 2004).
    One case involved the gang attack and killing of an
    older male, Ntologi, who had been a particularly
    despotic alpha male of the Mahale M community for years
    (Nishida, 1996). In two of the cases young adult males
    who had not formed good friendships within the
    community, and were highly aggressive toward older males
    were beaten, bitten, kicked and drug, until their wounds
    were so severe that they didn't survive (Fawcett &
    Muhumza, 2000; Watts, 2004).

    On October 29, 2002 David Watts was observing males from
    the Ngogo chimpanzee community in Kibale National Park
    in western Uganda, when he observed a gang of adult
    males attack and kill a young adult male named Grapelli,
    from their own community. I had spent a lot of time with
    Grapelli over the previous two years, and had gotten to
    know him fairly well during that time. He was a striking
    example of a young male chimpanzee, with distinctive
    diagonal black markings on a rare, light tan face. He
    was also one of the biggest, most aggressive chimpanzees
    at Ngogo and didn't spend much time with the older,
    higher ranking males of the community. Instead, Grapelli
    would go off by himself, for weeks on end, and when he
    returned he would fight with the other males. Between
    when Professor Watts left the party of chimpanzees on
    the night of the 28th and when he caught back up with
    them on the morning of the 29th, something had snapped
    in the other males. When he arrived on the scene, the
    attack was already underway, and a large group of adult
    males was repeatedly attacking Grapelli, pulling,
    punching, kicking, dragging and biting him, until he was
    bloodied and struggling for breath. Grapelli was beaten
    so badly during the attack that he could barely manage
    to pull himself into a rudely constructed nest in a low
    treetop before collapsing. The next day he was missing
    and it took another eight months before his decomposed
    body was discovered by two of the Ngogo field
    assistants.

    In all three instances the males that were killed
    appeared to have broken social rules or norms, and
    bullying-like behaviors that erupted into violence were
    used to attempt to get them to conform. Among
    chimpanzee, and many other primate societies, proper
    socialization and conformity are critical for
    maintaining social order and consistency, just as they
    are in humans. Individuals whose behavior challenges,
    disrupts or are considered unusual are often the targets
    of aggression, and that aggression continues until those
    individuals change their behavior. Bullying-like
    behaviors are not only present in many primate species,
    they are often utilized to accomplish the same goals.
    Bullying-like behaviors are used to enhance an
    individual or coalition's competitive ability, or to
    coerce others into changing their behavior to conform to
    the rest of the community. Bullying-like behaviors
    provide the individuals who engage in them with
    advantages over their targets, through enhanced status
    or access to resources, or both. If this sounds
    familiar, it's because humans use bullying behaviors to
    achieve the same ends.

    The major differences between the bullying-like
    behaviors so common in other primates and animals and
    the bullying that is plaguing the young children of the
    US and other countries are some of the very traits that
    are hallmarks of humanity. Humans have taken an ancient
    behavior that used to provide an advantage in survival
    and reproduction and altered its intensity and impact
    through language and culture. While physical bullying is
    a serious issue and targets of bullying are beaten all
    too often, humans have intensified and expanded the
    impact of bullying by incorporating language. Language
    allows us to communicate abstract ideas, coordinate
    behaviors and express thoughts and feelings to others.
    Language also allows us to gossip, and gossiping is a
    key psychological element in bullying and can have
    serious, lasting effects (Sharp, 1995).

    Language, combined with a phenomenal social memory that
    allows us to remember scores of individuals and their
    attributes, which we inherited from our primate
    ancestors, allows bullies to spread rumors about their
    targets, and inflict harm on them, without putting
    themselves at risk, physically. Text and online bullying
    are extensions of this behavior and further remove the
    bullies themselves from immediate risk. It is not
    anonymity that texting and online interactions provide,
    but rather the opportunity for individuals to distance
    themselves from potential conflict and risk that
    provides them with a platform to be cruel.

    Humans have further altered the impact of bullying-like
    behaviors through cultural practices and norms that
    celebrate violence and demand conformity to a narrow
    view of what is acceptable and normal. In the multi-
    national study mentioned earlier, the most intensive
    bullying was found in countries where violence and
    social intolerance are the most commonplace (Due et al,
    2005). In the US, views on violence, sexuality and what
    is normal impact the actions of our youth, and play on
    our inherent tendencies to coerce others into
    conformity. We know that humans are incredibly
    susceptible to suggestion from authority figures and are
    willing to commit what would otherwise be considered
    heinous crimes when directed or encouraged to by
    authority figures (Milgram, 1974). Still, cultures do
    not "create" bullies and bullies are not found only in
    those cultures that practice social intolerance and
    glorify violence. The tendency to bully, or coerce,
    others is natural and deeply rooted in our evolutionary
    history, and emerges in any group of toddlers playing
    freely. However, when cultures condone and in some cases
    celebrate violence and aggression, while suppressing or
    demonizing aspects of humanity that are equally natural
    such as homosexuality, they unwittingly give license to
    and encourage bullies.

    Bullying was there during the birth of our species
    having been inherited from the earliest of our social
    ancestors. Species ranging from rats to chimpanzees
    regularly engage in bullying-like behaviors, and those
    behaviors provide advantages to the individuals who
    engage in them. However, the combinatory effects of
    language and culture on bullying in humans have
    distorted its effects, pushing it beyond individually
    advantageous to socially venomous. The result has been
    the crisis we see played out in our schools, shopping
    malls and social media websites, children and young
    adults bullying each other with devastating results.
    While nearly all anti-bullying programs are well-meaning
    and can show progress in the short term, they fail to
    get at the root of the problem. Addressing bullying
    through culturally based social programs is like taking
    the flowerhead off a milk thistle. You will slow the
    growth and spread of the plant, but not for long. It is
    only through incorporating a deeper understanding of the
    antiquity of a behavior like bullying in our policies
    that we can hope to alter its impact on society. Like
    milk thistle, bullying must be pulled up by the root if
    we hope to remove it from the fields where our children
    grow and develop.

    References:

    Altmann, J. 1980. Baboon Mothers and Infants. University
    of Chicago Press.

    Boehm, C. 1999. Hierarchy in the Forest: The Evolution
    of Egalitarian Behavior, Cambridge: Harvard University
    Press.

    Cosmides, L & Tooby, J. 1990 Evolutionary Psychology: A
    Primer.

    Due, P, Holstein, B, Lynch, J, Diderichsen, F, Gabhain,
    S, Scheidt, P, Currie, C, and The Health Behaviour in
    School-Aged Children Bullying Working Group* .2005.
    Bullying and symptoms among school-aged children:
    international comparative cross sectional study in 28
    countries. European Journal of Public Health, Vol. 15,
    No. 2, 128-132.

    Fawcett, K. & Muhumza, G. 2000. Death of a Wild
    Chimpanzee Community Member: Possible Outcome of Intense
    Sexual Competition. American Journal of Primatology
    51:243-247.

    Goodall, J. 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of
    Behavior. Harvard University Press.

    Kinsey, S, Bailey, M, Sheridan, J, Padgett, D, Avitsur,
    R. 2007. Repeated Social Defeat Causes Increased
    Anxiety-Like Behavior and Alters Splenocyte Function in
    C57BL/6 and CD-1 Mice. Brain Behav Immun. May; 21(4):
    458-466.

    Milgram, S. 1974. Obedience to Authority: An
    Experimental View. Harper.

    Nansel, T, Overpeck, M, Pilla, R, Ruan, WJ, Simons-
    Morton, B, Scheidt, P. 2001. Bullying Behaviors Among US
    Youth: Prevalence and Association With Psychosocial
    Adjustment. JAMA. 2001;285(16):2094-2100.

    Nishida, T. 1996. The Death of Ntologi, The Unparalleled
    Leader of M Group. Pan African News. Vol.3, No.1

    Sapolsky, R. M. 1987. Stress, social status, and
    reproductive physiology in free-living baboons.
    Psychobiology of reproductive behavior: An evolutionary
    perspective. In: Psychobiology of reproductive behavior:
    An evolutionary perspective. Crews, David (Ed), pp.
    291-322. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, US: Prentice-Hall, Inc,
    xii, 350 pp.

    Seyfarth, R. 1976. Social relationships among adult
    female baboons. Animal Behaviour 24, 917-938.

    Sharp, S. 1995. How much does bullying hurt? The effects
    of bullying on the personal wellbeing and educational
    progress of secondary aged students. Educational and
    Child Psychology, Vol 12(2), 81-88.

    Sherrow, H. M. 2008. Variation in and ontogeny of social
    behavior in young male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes
    schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda.
    Ph.D. Thesis. Yale University.

    Smith, P, Cowie, H, Olafsson, R, & Liefooghe, A. 2002.
    Definitions of bullying: A comparison of terms used, and
    age and sex differences, in a 14-country international
    comparison. Child Development, 73, 1119-1133.

    Turnbull, C. 1961. The Forest People. Simon & Schuster.

    Vidal, J, Buwalda, B, Koolhaas, J. 2011. Differential
    long-term effects of social stress during adolescence on
    anxiety in Wistar and wild-type rats. Behavioural
    Processes, Volume 87, Issue 2, June 2011, Pages 176-182.

    Wang, J, Iannotti, R, Nansel, T. 2009. School Bullying
    Among Adolescents in the United States: Physical,
    Verbal, Relational, and Cyber. Journal of Adolescent
    Health, Volume 45, Issue 4, 368-375.

    Watts, D. 2004. Intracommunity coalitionary killing of
    an adult male chimpanzee at Ngogo, Kibale National Park,
    Uganda. Int J Primatol 25: 507-521.
    | Login or Register (free) to reply publicly or privately   Email

  2. Gratitude expressed by 4 members:

  3. TopTop #2
    CSummer's Avatar
    CSummer
     

    Re: The Origins of Bullying

    This was longer than I cared to read in its entirety, and I was more than a bit disenchanted by this conclusion, based on a study done in 28 countries:

    "Despite the variation in the amount and intention
    of bullying across human cultures one thing is clear,
    bullying is everywhere. The universality of bullying
    across human societies indicates that this is a species-
    typical human behavior that has little to do with the
    cultures people live in. Bullying, it seems is part of
    our normal behavioral repertoire, it is part of the
    human condition."

    28 is a pretty small minority of countries in a world of around 190 nations. And within those countries, how many different subcultures were examined? The Ladakhis of northern India? Indigenous tribes of the Amazon?

    My sense is that we in modern "civilized" societies - and even in some indigenous cultures - "swim" in an ocean of humans whose consciousness is fragmented to at least some degree, and a pervasive element of that fragmentation is an unconscious sense of powerlessness. I would guess that the rare people who suffer minimally - or not at all - from such inner fragmenting are not inclined to bully others and that bullying is simply an outward projection and rejection of one's own sense of weakness (very similar to - or a form of - scapegoating). I would also guess that one would have to look a long time - or know where to look - to find people who were free enough of such inner wounds that they didn't pass them on to their offspring.

    It seems important to me to question such conclusions as are in this article as too many negative traits have been ascribed to "human nature" that are actually a manifestation of human psychological impairment that we consider "normal." I wonder if many such traits might not be nonexistent in a truly healthy culture.

    C Summer

    | Login or Register (free) to reply publicly or privately   Email

  4. Gratitude expressed by 2 members:

Similar Threads

  1. Replies: 4
    Last Post: 11-15-2011, 09:00 AM
  2. President Obama Doesn't Understand the Origins of the Deficit
    By Karl Frederick in forum National & International Politics
    Replies: 4
    Last Post: 07-29-2011, 03:13 AM
  3. Origins of Mothers' Day
    By sharingwisdom in forum WaccoReader
    Replies: 1
    Last Post: 03-03-2010, 06:35 AM
  4. Origins of the Moonwalk
    By Barry in forum WaccoReader
    Replies: 3
    Last Post: 07-15-2009, 08:37 AM
  5. The Origins of Gambling - World History
    By Dark Shadows in forum WaccoReader
    Replies: 0
    Last Post: 02-03-2008, 12:24 AM

Bookmarks